Skip to Main Content

Is Speed Reading Bullshit?

Is Speed Reading Bullshit?
Credit: Lifehacker (Photos: Getty Images) - In-House Art

Speed reading is a skill that almost seems like a superpower. The ability to quickly read and comprehend books, articles and other written materials would be life-changing for a lot of us. But is speed reading something you’re born having the ability to do, or is it something anyone can learn? A slew of cheap apps claiming to teach the technique would like you to think that it is teachable. Let’s take a look at the claims of speed reading and if it’s really possible to read 1,200 words a minute.

For some context: most of us tend to read at about 200-400 words per minute. Speed readers claim to hit around 1000-1700 words per minute. To get a better idea of whether these claims have research to back them up, Lifehacker spoke with professor and eye tracking expert Keith Rayner, Ph.D. from the University of California, San Diego shortly before he passed away in 2015.

Let’s start by taking a look at different methods of speed reading before we dig into what does and doesn’t work about it.

Different methods of speed reading

Before we can talk about speed reading, we need to lay down the specifics of how we tend to read and comprehend text normally. Slate breaks down what we know about reading:

When you factor out the amount of time spent thinking through complex and unfamiliar concepts—a rarity when people read for pleasure—reading is an appallingly mechanical process. You look at a word or several words. This is called a “fixation,” and it takes about .25 seconds on average. You move your eye to the next word or group of words. This is called a “saccade,” and it takes up to about .1 seconds on average. After this is repeated once or twice, you pause to comprehend the phrase you just looked at. That takes roughly 0.3 to 0.5 seconds on average. Add all these fixations and saccades and comprehension pauses together and you end up with about 95 percent of all college-level readers reading between 200 and 400 words per minute.

Speed readers supposedly shorten how long they fixate on a word. They tend to do this by cutting down on subvocalization, the end goal being faster than the 0.3-0.5 seconds listed above. The idea of speed reading has been around since the 1950s, but it’s seeing a revival recently with many apps popping up in the past few years.

As you’d expect, there are a few different popular methods of speed reading, but most fall into a couple broad methods, include skimming, meta guiding, Rapid Serial Visual Presentation and others.

Skimming is glancing through text to find important parts to read. Although this is usually considered a speed reading method, you’re not teaching yourself to read faster. You’re just learning which parts you can skip over. As you’d expect, studies show that we don’t remember that many details when we skim.

Meta guiding is an older technique. It’s when you use a finger (or a pointer like a pen) to guide your eyes. The point is to decrease distraction and focus on the specific words to increase your reading speed.

Another method pushes you “read” multiple lines at once by expanding your peripheral vision. Tim Ferriss has developed a speed reading technique that riffs on this idea and trains your focus more than anything else.

Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) is a method used by most of the recent digital speed reading systems. Single words flash on the screen, so you’re concentrating on a single word at a time. As you get used to the system, you speed up how fast the display shows you words. You can see an RSVP method called Spritz in action here.

If you’re curious about your own reading speed, you can find yours with this test from productivity site ReadWriteWork or this one from MyReadSpeed.com.

How speed reading affects comprehension

Sure, speed reading is a nice idea, and the ability to see 1,000 words a minute is technically possible. But the problem is that you won’t truly understand all of those words. Research is pretty limited here, but a book Rayner edited, “Eye Movements and Information Processing During Reading” is one of the more comprehensive looks at how our eyes work when we’re reading. Rayner believes speed reading claims are nonsense because our eyes can’t really work that way. As he explained in our interview:

You can probably push yourself to get a little over 500 words per minute, but you’re limited by the eyes and the anatomy of the retina. To understand text you need to move your eyes to put the fovea on the part of the text you want to focus. Acuity drops off pretty markedly outside the fovea and you can’t discriminate the words and text far from the fovea. So, that’s the rate limited factor, as is how fast the brain can process information.

When it comes to eliminating subvocalization with techniques like meta guiding, Rayner pointed out you quickly lose comprehension:

You can practice going faster and you probably will, but when you start going too fast you’ll start losing comprehension. Most speed reading methods involve getting rid of subvocalization.

Research shows

that when you do that and the text is difficult,

comprehension goes to pieces

.

As for methods like Ferriss’ that require you to read multiple lines at once? Rayner said they don’t work:

The other claim is that you can take in more information per eye fixation, but there’s no evidence that says we can do that. What we know about the physiology of the retina is against the notion that you can take in two lines of text at the same time.

And finally, the digital system, RSVP, has a serious problem with working memory:

Then with RSVP, words come pretty fast, but working memory gets overloaded and words come in faster than you can deal with them.

Studies—including this 2016 research by Rayner and his colleagues—show that as reading speed increases, comprehension drops. This means you’re not taking in the information, which defeats the purpose of reading. With RSVP (and the apps that use it) you don’t have the ability to look back to reread text and you overload short term memory so you don’t remember as much.

Of course, counter arguments exist. Most recently, Spritz has made claims that counter Rayner’s assertions. This is hardly surprising given that they are selling an app to train people to speed read. Spritz claims that since their system allows your eyes to rest on a single point, you can read faster. That could be true, but it doesn’t account for Rayner’s working memory problem.

Spritz also isn’t sharing how their research was done or how many people were part of their study. So that, coupled with the fact that they are selling a speed reading app means that you’ll want to take these findings with a grain (or spoonful) of salt. In fact, in our hunt for studies backing up speed reading claims, we found most research was conducted by the companies who sell various speed reading methods. In the case of Tim Ferriss’ technique, he’s using ideas grounded in science, but I couldn’t find research beyond Ferriss’ own claims on his blog post.

Apps that teach you speed reading

It’s not all bad news though. Speed reading has plenty of supporters. Slate’s Jim Pagels loves the RSVP system. The Atlantic’s Olga Khazan suggests that speed reading apps are best for boring news stories or your email, not anything that requires complete comprehension. Likewise, methods that rely on skimming instead of reading faster make sense when you’re cramming in school or reading something that isn’t important.

You can try it out for yourself easily these days. You won’t have to order any of these apps from the back of a sketchy magazine and most are just a couple bucks or free. Still, the bottom line is: don’t expect amazing results. Here are a few options:

  • Spreed: This Chrome extension uses the RSVP technique to flash words on your screen. Just select the text you want from any web page and Spreed does the rest. (Free)

  • Outread: An iOS app that uses a variation of meta guiding. (Free)

  • Accelerator: An iOS app that uses a variation of RSVP. ($2.99)

Ultimately, it’s up to you to decide how much time and effort you want to put into trying to read faster. It takes a lot of focus and mental effort to speed read, and when you do it you’re missing out on information. If you’re someone who likes to take a few moments to pause and think about an idea when you’re enjoying a book or an article, speed reaching might not be for you, because those moments are gone.

So, in short: Speed reading anything you need to truly comprehend is probably a bad idea. However, if you have a few documents you need to get through or you’re reading something that isn’t that important, these methods can still be worthwhile. Just know that you won’t become a super-fast reading comprehension machine.

This story was originally published in March 2014 and updated on October 28, 2020 to updated dead links, align the content with current Lifehacker style, revise the headline, and add a new header image..